(5.2.4.6) Family resemblance
The Intellectual Production of Engineers, p. 160, additions after (5.2.4.5) Benefits of reducing burden by classification.
---
https://gyazo.com/a3c832d9b96f39ed63d35fdeda59bfb5
Fig: A family
A "family" is a group. There are not necessarily characteristics that are common to all members of this group. Even if there are no characteristics that are common to all members, the whole group can be loosely connected by characteristics that are partially common to all members, forming a group.
The term "familial resemblance" was coined by Ludwig Wittgenstein. He considered whether groups of what the term "game" refers to share common characteristics and argued that not all games share common characteristics, but rather a complex web of similarities that overlap each other. He simply referred to groups connected by familial similarities as "families."
I explained it this way: "I am a
When you find one that seems to be related, move it so that the two warps are close to each other. By repeating this process, a group of warp lines that seem to be related to each other will gradually form.
The Intellectual Production of Engineers p.152
In classification, "characteristics common to a group" (criteria) are determined first, and the presence or absence of those characteristics determines whether a person is a member of the group or not. On the other hand, grouping in the KJ method requires only a relationship among individual members, not common characteristics. This is much like the idea of familial similarity.
It seems we need advice to release the captivity of "we must create a group with common characteristics."
-----
There is an interesting experiment regarding familial similarity.
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive science, 26(5), 653-684.
https://gyazo.com/4c60884f971a6a15541414557d2a0c70
Which group does the target object resemble? European Americans and East Asians gave diametrically opposite answers to this question.
https://gyazo.com/26318f028c04d1e3c3906a8c03c30b89
Let's delve into the two ways of thinking.
This flower has four attributes.
Attribute 1: Target petals are round, Group 1 has 3/4 petals round, Group 2 has 1/4 petals round
Attribute 2: Target flower center is a single circle, 3/4 of Group 1 is a single circle, 1/4 of Group 2 is a single circle
Attribute 3: Target has leaves on it, Group 1 has leaves on 3/4, Group 2 has leaves on 1/4
Attribute 4: Target stems are straight, Group 1 has 0/4 stems straight, Group 2 has 4/4 stems straight
In this situation, would you put the target in group 1 or group 2?
Rule-based: Attributes 1-3 do not clearly separate the groups. Attribute 4 is the criterion that separates the two groups, and if the criteria are followed, the group should be placed in Group 2.
Family resemblance-based: the idea that since three of the four attributes indicate that "group 1 is closer", it should be placed in group 1.
Interestingly, when the question is not which is similar, but which should be classified as similar, East Asian also makes a rule-based decision. In other words, East Asians consider classification and similarity to be different, while European Americans do not distinguish between the two.
Rule-based is easy to explain to others the reason for the decision. It can be a clear proposition: "If the stem is straight, it is group 2." On the other hand, it is vulnerable to noise. In this example, attribute 4 could cleanly separate the two groups, but if noise is present and even attribute 4 cannot separate the two groups, Rule-based falls into the trap of thinking that "there is no clear criterion to distinguish these two groups. It is also weak when some of the attributes are unobservable. If attribute 4 happens to be unobservable, the decision is reversed.
Family resembranse-based is resistant to noise and to unobservability. However, the reason for the decision cannot be simply explained. If we dare to explain, the rule would be: "If the petals are round, one vote for group 1; if the center of the flower is a single circle, one vote for group 1; if it has leaves, one vote for group 1; if the stem is straight, two votes for group 2. The rule is: "The group with the highest total number of votes shall be the group with the highest number of votes.
The KJ method was born in East Asia, Japan. Jiro Kawakita strongly insisted that it is not a classification. The idea of familial similarity may make it easier to understand for people of European descent.
---
This page is auto-translated from /nishio/(5.2.4.6) 家族的類似性 using DeepL. If you looks something interesting but the auto-translated English is not good enough to understand it, feel free to let me know at @nishio_en. I'm very happy to spread my thought to non-Japanese readers.
(5.2.4.6) Family resemblance
https://gyazo.com/a3c832d9b96f39ed63d35fdeda59bfb5
Fig: A family
"Family" is a group. Not all members of a family have common features. Even if there are no common features to all members, partially common features loosely connect members to form a group.
The word "family resemblance" was made by Ludwig Wittgenstein. He wondered if what was called a "game" had common features. He concluded that there is not a feature that is common to all games, but there is a mesh of complex similarities that overlap each other. He called the group connected by family resemblance "family."
In (5.2.3) Make related things close, I said:
After you spread the pieces, you look at them for a while. You find pieces that seem to be related. If you find those pieces, you move them so that those pieces are nearby. By repeating this, gradually a group of pieces seems to be related to one another gradually forms.
In classification, we determine "features common to groups" (criteria) in advance, and determines whether a piece is a member of the group based on the presence or absence of the features. On the other hand, in group organization of the KJ method, we focus there is a relationship between individual members and does not require common features. The concept of group organization is very similar to the concept of family resemblance.
You do not need to create a group with common features.
---
There is an interesting experiment on family resemblance. (*1)
(*1): Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J., & Nisbett, R. E. (2002). Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive science, 26(5), 653-684.
https://gyazo.com/4c60884f971a6a15541414557d2a0c70
Which group is the target object is most similar to? In response to this question, European Americans and East Asians gave the opposite answer.
https://gyazo.com/26318f028c04d1e3c3906a8c03c30b89
Let us think more detail to understand the thought opposite to you.
These flowers have 4 features:
Feature 1: Target petals are round.
The 3/4 flowers in group 1 have round petals.
The 1/4 flowers in group 2 have round petals.
Feature 2: The center of the target flower is a single circle, not doubled.
The 3/4 of group 1 have s single circle.
The 1/4 of group 2 have a single circle.
Feature 3: Target has a leaf.
The 3/4 of group 1 have a leaf.
The 1/4 of group 2 have a leaf.
Feature 4: Target stem is straight.
The 0/4 of group 1 have a straight stem.
The 4/4 of group 2 have a straight stem.
In this situation, which group is the left target object is most similar to?
There are two type of thinking:
Rule-based: Features 1 to 3 do not clearly separate groups. Feature 4 is the criteria to separate these two groups. The target has a straight stem. So it is similar to group 2.
Family resemblance-based: Features 1 to 3 say the target is more similar to group 1 than group 2. The 3 out of 4 features indicate that it is similar to group 1. So it is similar to group 1.
Another experiment asked, "which group the target object belongs to." In this case, East Asian also makes a rule-based decision. In other words, East Asian thinks that classification and similarity are different, but European American does not distinguish it.
Rule-based thought is easy to explain the reason for the decision to others. You can show a clear proposition that "if the stem is straight, it is group 2." On the other hand, it is weak to noise. In this example, we can separate two groups with feature 4 clearly. However, if there is a noise on feature 4, the rule-based people think that there is no clear criterion to distinguish the two groups. They stop thinking. The thought is also weak to the situation that some of the features are unobservable. If they can not observe feature 4, they also stop thinking.
Family resemblance-based thought is strong against noise and unobservability. However, they can not explain the reason for the judgment simply. One example of explanation is using vote:
If the target petals are round, one vote for group 1, otherwise one vote for group 2.
If the target has one center circle, one vote for group 1, otherwise one vote for group 2.
If the target has leaves, one vote for group 1, otherwise one vote for group 2.
If the target stem is straight, two votes for group 2, otherwise two votes for group 1.
Sum the number of votes to determine the group with the highest number.
The KJ method was born in East Asia, Japan. Kawakita Jiro strongly insisted what "it is not classification." The concept of family resemblance helps Europeans to understand the KJ method.
en.icon